Researchers map new area of language in the human brain.

For 140 years, scientists’ understanding of language comprehension in the brain came from individuals with stroke.  Based on language impairments caused by stroke, scientists believed a single area of the brain, a hotdog shaped section in the temporal lobe of the left hemisphere called Wernicke’s region, was the center of language comprehension. Wernicke’s was thought to be responsible for understanding the meaning of single words and sentences, two separate and critical functions.  Now, Northwestern Medicine researchers have updated and redrawn the traditional brain map of language comprehension based on new research with individuals who have a rare form of dementia that affects language, Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA).

The current study shows word comprehension is actually located in a different brain neighbourhood, the left anterior temporal lobe, a more forward location than Wernicke’s. And sentence comprehension turns out to be distributed widely throughout the language network, not in a single area as previously thought.  The team states that these new findings provides an important change in the overall understanding of language comprehension in the brain.  They go on to add that knowing where language comprehension is located offers a more precise target for future therapies that could potentially protect or restore language function.

Previous studies show that strokes cut off blood supply to regions of the brain and cause destruction of both neurons and fiber pathways passing through that region.  In the 1870s, a scientist named Carl Wernicke observed a specific region damaged by stroke and resulting language impairments. This area, consequently named Wernicke’s region, was identified as the seat of language comprehension.  The historical studies states that people who had strokes that affected Wernicke’s region couldn’t explain what a word such as umbrella meant.  Secondly, they had difficulty understanding sentence construction. If they were told, ‘Put the apple on top of the book,’ even if they understood the meaning of apple and book, they wouldn’t be able to carry out the command because they can’t understand the construction of the sentence.

However, the researchers had been puzzling over the fact that their PPA patients with damage in Wernicke’s area did not have the word comprehension impairment seen in stroke patients. They still understood individual words. And their sentence comprehension was inconsistent; some understood sentences; some didn’t.  It was becoming clear over the many years the researchers treated these patients that there was some disconnect between what textbooks said and what was observed in their patients. The current study was performed to analyze this discrepancy. The view of brain as seen from stroke did not match the view of the brain when seen from PPA.

The researchers began conducting quantitative MRI imaging of their PPA patients’ brains and testing their language.  The current study consists of the imaging of 72 PPA patients with damage inside and outside of Wernicke’s area. The team measured cortex thickness in all of these areas. The lab explain that cortex thickness is an indirect measure of the number of neurons and brain health. Thinning of the cortex in PPA indicates the destruction of neurons by the disease.

The data findings showed that PPA patients who lost cortical thickness in Wernicke’s area still could understand individual words and had varied impairment of sentence comprehension. None of these patients had the global type of comprehension impairment described in stroke patients with Wernicke’s aphasia.  The results also showed that severe word comprehension loss was only seen in PPA patients who had diminished cortical thickness in a region of the brain completely outside of Wernicke’s area, in the front part of the temporal lobe. This part of the brain is not prone to the effects of stroke, so its role in comprehension had been missed in prior language maps.

The researchers note that the discrepancy between the traditional map of comprehension and what was seen in PPA can be explained by the different ways the two diseases injure Wernicke’s area. In PPA, the neurodegenerative disease does not destroy the underlying fiber pathways that allow language areas to work together. But, in stroke patients, those critical highways passing through Wernicke’s had been blown up. So, the messages from other parts of the brain to the left anterior temporal lobe, the spot for word comprehension, were simply not getting through.

The team surmise that in this case, a different map of language was uncovered by comparing two different models of disease, one based on strokes that destroy an entire region of brain, cortex as well as underlying pathways, and the other on a neurodegenerative disease that attacks mostly brain cells in cortex rather than the region as a whole.  They go on to conclude that the study overall has provided a more precise brain target for future therapies to restore language.

Source:  Northwestern University

 

The Neuroscience of Sapience .  The internal conversation loop (phonological loop) involves the listening (hearing language processing) in Wernicke's area receiving echoes from the speech production area of the frontal lobe, Brocca's area. The same channels from Brocca's area, along with control signals from the cerebellum, go to the vocal chords, tongue, diaphragm and other voicing muscles, but are presumably blocked when you are thinking to yourself. When you are talking to yourself you probably get both actual sound inputs along with internal (non-voiced) inputs in Wernicke's area.  Credit:  George Mobus.  The University of Washington Tacoma, Institute of Technology.
The Neuroscience of Sapience . The internal conversation loop (phonological loop) involves the listening (hearing language processing) in Wernicke’s area receiving echoes from the speech production area of the frontal lobe, Brocca’s area. The same channels from Brocca’s area, along with control signals from the cerebellum, go to the vocal chords, tongue, diaphragm and other voicing muscles, but are presumably blocked when you are thinking to yourself. When you are talking to yourself you probably get both actual sound inputs along with internal (non-voiced) inputs in Wernicke’s area. Credit: George Mobus. The University of Washington Tacoma, Institute of Technology.

One thought on “Researchers map new area of language in the human brain.

Leave a Reply

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.